Conclusion

AuthorMcNally, Sandra
Pages33-34
33
address the perceptions of ability by inculcating a ‘growth mindset’ in pedagogical
practice throughout schooli ng. In maths, thi s is the extent to which individuals believe
that their maths abilities can be improved over time with effort, as opposed to being
unchangeable. It has been shown to be particularly efficacious for girls (Boal ar, 2013).
Secondly, girls do respond well to female role models and this appears to be
especially the case for girls of higher ability. While it may not be practical (or desirable)
to change educational systems to be segregated along gender-specific lines, it would be
very interesting to stu dy whether setting of high-ability students along the lines of
gender in particular subjects might influence performance and future course-taking.21
Given the strong effects of rol e models across most of the literature, it woul d seem to
be i mportant to address this both i n schools and in tertiary education. This i s also a
conclusion of Zafar (2013) who finds that most of the difference in STEM at the tertiary
level is driven by gender differences in tastes and preferences. He concludes a possible
policy implication…is to encourage policies that increase the representation of females
in academic science and engineering, since these female professors may change female
students’ beliefs and preferences toward STEM coursework and careers. Cheryan et al.
(2015) emphasise the importance of di versifying the i mage of computer science and
engineering. They argue that girls are currently exposed to an unrealistic image of these
fields that depicts all computer science and engineering cultures as fitting a narrow
profile. They argue that as mor e women and girls are welcomed into these fields, the
process of culture change will likely build on itself.
There are numerous in terventions to encourage more girls and women to enter
and stay in STEM fields. As argued by Cheryan et al. (2017) and also the Skills
Commission in the UK (2019), much more needs to be done to evaluate programmes in
a scientific manner and to collate and diss eminate results. Cheryan et al. (2017) and
Cheryan et al. (2015) report very heartening case studies of a number of university
departments in the US that made changes to their computer science departments with
very impressive subsequent increases in the percentage of female computer science
graduates within a decade (i.e. Harvey Mudd, Carnegie Mellon and University of
Washington).22 As described succinctly by Cheryan et al. (2015), in addition to structural
changes (e.g. recruitment procedures), these programmes changed stereotypes of
computer science by u sing diverse role mod els, exposing students to a wide range of
applications for computer science and revamping their introductory course so that it was
no longer seen as a field for ‘geeky know-it-alls’. These examples show that it is possible
to put a combination of policies in place an d have big effects on enrolment. Thus, the
gender gap in STEM in tertiary education may be prominent in many different countries,
and often for similar reasons. But it is not inevitable. Culture can change.
10. Conclusion
The S TEM g ender gap in tertiary education results from factors that influence
educational preparedness as well as facto rs that influence tho se who are ‘STEM ready’
at the point of making choices within tertiary education. These factors are often similar,
such as lack of confidence among females (particularly with regard to maths ability) and
lack of ‘female f riendliness’ of educational environments even within upper secondary
education (see Table 1). The comm on stereotyping of male and female abili ties as well
as the stereotyping of particular fields of study seem to have a lot to do wi th this.
21 ‘Setting’ along the lines of ability is common within secondary schools in the UK. This is where students of
different abilities are taught in separate classes for particular subjects (e.g. maths) but not for others.
22 The percentage of female computer science graduates increased from under 10 per cent to 40 per cent at
Harvey Mudd and Carnegie Mellon; and from 15 per cent to 30 per cent at the University of Washington).

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT