Financial Professionals' Overconfidence: Is It Experience, Function, or Attitude?

Published date01 March 2014
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-036X.2011.00636.x
Date01 March 2014
European Financial Management, Vol. 20, No. 2, 2014, 236–269
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-036X.2011.00636.x
Financial Professionals’
Overconfidence: Is It Experience,
Function, or Attitude?
Oliver Gloede
Department of Economics, Leibniz Universit¨
at Hannover, K ¨
onigsworther Platz 1, 30167 Hannover,
Germany
E-mail: gloede@gif.uni-hannover.de
Lukas Menkhoff
Department of Economics, Leibniz Universit¨
at Hannover, K ¨
onigsworther Platz 1, 30167 Hannover,
Germany, and Centre forEuropean Economic Research (ZEW) in Mannheim, Germany
E-mail: menkhoff@gif.uni-hannover.de
Abstract
This paper examines financial professionals’ overconfidence in their forecasting
performance. We compare individuals’ self-rating of performance with the true
performance, both measured relative to the same peer group. The forecasters
in our sample show overconfidence on average, although to a moderate degree,
including many cases of underconfidence. In analysing this, we find that working
experience is accompanied by less overconfidence. Function is also related to less
overconfidence, such as being a fund manager and using fundamental analysis.
The same effect is found for the attitude to herd, whereas recent success appears
with more overconfident professionals.
Keywords: better-than-average,self-rating,forecasting,foreign exchange,
performance
JEL classification: G1, D84, F31
We thank participants at the annual conferences of the European Financial Management
Association, German Economic Association, German Finance Association, Money Macro
Finance Research Group, and the RoyalEconomic Society, as well as the audiences of several
seminars, in particular Michael Br¨
auninger, Jerry Coakley, Michael Frenkel,Markus Glaser,
Alan Kirman, Thomas Lux, Christian Pfeifer, Winfried Pohlmeier, Michael Schr ¨
oder and
Philipp Sibbertsen for their helpful comments and discussion. We gratefully acknowledge
very constructive comments by the editor and two anonymousreferees. In addition, we thank
the Centre for European Economic Research (ZEW) in Mannheim, Germany, for providing
the data set. Oliver Gloede gratefully acknowledges a scholarship of the Foundation of
German Business (Stiftung der Deutschen Wirtschaft).
C
2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Financial Professionals’ Overconfi dence 237
1. Introduction
We know from a series of, by now, ‘classical’ studies that most people are overconf ident
regarding their own abilities, such as their driving performance (Svenson, 1981).
Subsequent research has shown that overconfidence is not an invariant characteristic
but also depends on circumstances. What is important in this respect is the feedback
that people receive, which seems helpful in adjusting one’s own perception. Financial
markets provide frequent and precise feedback to their participants, so that overconfident
behaviour may be less expected here. In addition, financial markets punish overcon-
fidence, as overconfidence reduces an investor’s performance (Odean, 1998). Given
the lack of research in this realm, exploring the extent of overconfidence of financial
professionals and its potential determinants seems warranted.
Our study contributes to this issue and is the first one to examine determinants
of the better-than-average (BTA) variant of overconfidence in the case of financial
professionals. Accordingly, we need two ingredients for this study, i.e., a BTA-measure
of overconfidence and the linkage of this measure to potential determinants. First, the
BTA-measure of overconf idence is the difference between a professional’s self-rated
relative performance with respect to a reference group and the same person’s true
relative performance within the same reference group. This precise measure is usually
not available. Second, we analyse relationships between individual forecasters’ overcon-
fidence and their demographic, (job) function and forecasting characteristics. Findings
reported here enrich our understanding about the determinants of overconfidence among
financial professionals, perhaps facilitating the introduction of measures for reducing
overconfidence and its costly consequences.
Our research uses a sample of 105 professional forecasters who are regularly
contributing to an established financial market survey in Germany. From this survey,
we gather monthly forecasts for the USD/DM and (later) USD/EUR exchange rate
over several years, so that we can calculate a meaningful forecasting performance. We
complement this performance information with data from additional surveys conducted
at the same time as the regular surveys. These supplementary surveys reveal the
professionals’ self-rating and further characteristics about them.
The financial professionals in this study are experienced, educated and hold senior
positions in the financial industry so that the sample seems to be relevant for our
purpose. Based on this sample, the BTA-measure of overconfidence shows reasonable
attributes (which will be related to the literature below). First, these professionals regard
themselves mostly better in their self-rating than their fellow forecasters and thus show
overconfidence on average. Second, the average degree of overconfidence is moderate,
which may be expected for professionals whoreceive frequent feedback and in a market,
foreign exchange, where forecasting is particularly difficult. Third, the combination
of moderate average overconfidence with substantial individual heterogeneity implies
that many professionals show underconfidence. Fourth, the self-rating of financial
professionals is not significantly related to the same person’s performance, indicating
that a correct self-rating is not easy. Fifth, our BTA-measure of overconfidence has the
expected correlations to alternative measures of overconfidence and thus seems reliable.
Below we document the following determinants of overconf idence. The working
experience of a professional is tentatively related to less overconf idence, suggesting that
experience helps in assessing one’s true performance. Also, two function-related criteria
are related to less overconfidence: first, being a fund manager, which may be due to
the immediate feedback that these professionals receive; and second, a heavy reliance
C
2011 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Oliver Gloede and Lukas Menkhoff
238
on fundamental analysis, which may indicate these professionals’ remarkable effor ts
in forecasting. Finally, a professional’s tendency to herd in his forecasts is associated
with less overconfidence. Thus, herding may be interpreted as a sign of less confidence
and possibly as a rational reaction to inferior information or ability. In contrast to the
relations just discussed,recent forecasting success is positively related to overconfidence,
with success inducing higher self-evaluation. These patterns provide obviouslessons for
financial professionals and their superiors.
This paper is structured as follows: Section 2 reviews the previous literature, showing
that our approach is original. Section 3 introduces the data used. Section 4 details
relationships between self-rating, overconfidence and performance. Section 5 provides
the main findings from regression analyses. And Section 6 concludes.
2. Literature
The aim of this study is to analyse overconfidence of professionals in financial markets.
Even though financial professionals are not the main subjects in empirical studies, we can
learn about determinants of overconfidence from the finance and psychology literature
which deals with overconfidence and BTA in general. A survey of the BTA literature is
provided by Alicke and Govorun (2005).
Overconfidence biases are expected to ease when tasks involve frequent feedback,
as is the case for financial markets (Lichtenstein and Fischhoff, 1980). The effect of
training could be dampened if these biases are deeply rooted in personality (see Preston
and Harris, 1965; Brehmer, 1980; Menkhoff and Nikiforow, 2009). Consequently, one
should expect a lower degree of overconf idence here than elsewhere. However, the
impact of feedback on behaviour requires that it is asked for and understood (Kruger
and Dunning, 1999). In this respect, there may be differences between various groups
in financial markets: professionals in particular might use and be able to interpret the
feedback they get. In contrast, non-professional participants, such as individual investors,
may have lower incentives to use feedback and may have less ability to interpret it.1
Available studies do support the notion that there is also overconfidence in f inancial
markets, but this evidence refers mainly to non-professionals or to financial markets
in general, where non-professionals are included. Specifically, there are three kinds of
studies. First, one stream of the literature relies on the theoretically-derived finding
that overconfidence of financial market participants can be detected by their increased
trading activity (Odean, 1998). There is ample evidence of ‘too much trading volume’
in financial markets in general (e.g., Statman et al., 2006). Second, other studies use
information about individual investors which is collected from their trading accounts
(among others are Odean, 1999; Glaser and Weber, 2007a; Grinblatt and Keloharju,
2009). Third, overconf idence is shown in experimentssimulating f inancial markets (e.g.,
Biais et al., 2005; Deaves et al., 2009). Thus, we know from these various perspectives
that overconfident behaviour appears in financial markets, but we know little about
whether financial market professionals also show this overconfident behaviour.
As professionals trade the largest volumes and have the best information among
financial market participants, evidence on their behaviour is of particular interest.
1In fact, professionals might even need a reasonable level of overconf idence to sustain their
optimistic and risk-loving attitude after failure, which is apparentlya warranted characteristic
of the financial industry (Taylor and Brown, 1988; Oberlechner and Osler, forthcoming).
C
2011 Blackwell Publishin
g
Ltd

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT