The face of risk: CEO facial masculinity and firm risk

Published date01 March 2019
Date01 March 2019
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/eufm.12175
Eur Financ Manag. 2019;25:239–270. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/eufm © 2018 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 239
DOI: 10.1111/eufm.12175
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
The face of risk: CEO facial masculinity and
firm risk
Shinichi Kamiya
1
|
Y. Han (Andy) Kim
2
|
Soohyun Park
3
1
Nanyang Business School, Nanyang
Technological University, S3-B1B-64,
Nanyang Avenue, Singapore 639798
Email: skamiya@ntu.edu.sg
2
SungKyunKwan University (SKKU),
Room 422, Business School Building,
Jongno-gu, Seoul 03063, Korea
Email: ayhkim@skku.edu
3
SungKyunKwan University (SKKU),
#90206, International Hall, Jongno-gu,
Seoul 03063, Korea
Email: tngusqkr1@skku.edu
Abstract
We examine whether a male CEO's facial masculinity,
measured by facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR), predicts
the riskiness of his firm. Using the face pictures of 1,162
CEOs in the Execucomp database, we find supporting
evidence. Firms with more masculine-faced CEOs have
higher stock return volatility and higher financial leverage
and are more acquisitive. Their frequency of acquisitions,
the dollar amount spent on acquisitions, and the takeover
premium are all higher. We find that more masculine-faced
CEOscompensation is more sensitive to the risk of the
firm. The result is robust when we use AI (artificial
intelligence)-measured fWHR of the CEOs.
KEYWORDS
CEO, fWHR, leverage, M&A, masculinity, risk, testosterone, VEGA
JEL CLASSIFICATION
G02, G32, G34, M1, Z1
We thank thank John Doukas, the editor, and the anonymous referee of European Financial Management for giving us
great comments to improve the quality of our paper. We thank the session participants at the AFA 2017 in Chicago, FMA
Asian Conference in Seoul, 2015, Korea Finance Association Annual Meeting 2015, Cambridge University, SKKU,
University of Edinburgh, and UNIST. We thank Malcolm Baker, Jong-bom Chay, Henrik Cronqvist, Stephen Dimmock,
John Griffin, Umit Gurun, Gerard Hoberg, Jarrad Harford, Mark Humphrey-Jenner, Jennifer Lerner, Christian Leuz, Angie
Low, Jun-Koo Kang, Ulrike Malmendier, Chris Parsons, Richard Roll, Burkard C. Schipper (AFA discussant), and
Jungwon Seo for insightful comments. We also thank the great research assistantship of Yujing Ma, Matthew Lim, Nico
Dharmawan, You Ye, Li Rui, Yong Mei Ling, Loo Pei Yu, Liew SiQi, Fam Suen Li, Goh Hui Min, Youngjae Jay Choi,
Tae Hoon Jeon, Seung Deok Shin, Liang Sihui, Lim Shi Bin, Goh Le Min, and Xu Fuqui. All errors are our own. Our
Python code is available upon request.
2
|
KAMIYA ET AL.
K
240
1
|
INTRODUCTION
In the upper echelons of corporate management, the literature stemming from Bertrand and Schoar
(2003) finds a significant impact of the traits of the CEO on firm decision-making. On the behavioral
front, the association between CEO overconfidence and corporate performance has been widely
studied. For example, the impact of CEO overconfidence or self-attribution bias on corporate
investment (Ben-David, Graham, & Harvey, 2013; Malmendier & Tate, 2005), acquisitions (Doukas &
Petmezas, 2007; Kim, 2013; Malmendier & Tate, 2008), innovation (Hirshleifer, Low, & Teoh, 2012),
and many other areas have been examined. Recently, researchers found that the biological traits of the
CEOs, especially, facial masculinity, are linked to corporate financial policies and performance
(Gomulya, Wong, Ormiston, & Boeker, 2016; Jia, van Lent, & Zeng, 2014; Wong, Ormiston, &
Haselhuhn, 2011).
1
Recent literature in neuroendocrinology established that men's facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR,
hereafter) predicts masculine social behaviors that are linked to occupying dominant positions under
competition. Carré and McCormick (2008) and Christiansen and Winkler (1992) find that high
fWHR is associated with more aggressive behavior. Campbell et al. (2010) find that fWHR predicts
sensation-seeking behavior. Haselhuhn and Wong (2011) find that high-fWHR persons are more likely
to cheat their negotiation partners for financial gain. Thus, Stirrat and Perrett (2010) find that men with
high fWHR are perceived as less trustworthy. However, Stirrat and Perrett (2012) find that in an
inter-group competition setting, the men with high fWHR cooperate more with other in-group
members and show more self-sacrificing cooperation to win the competition against the other groups.
Hence, Wong et al. (2011) find that male CEOs with high fWHR achieve better financial performance
than male CEOs with low fWHR do. Jia et al. (2014) find that masculine-faced CEOs are more likely to
misreport accounting statements. Gomulya et al. (2016) interpret the masculine face as less trustworthy
and find that executives with high fWHR are less likely to be appointed as the CEO after the dismissals
of CEOs with financial misconduct. Extending the literature, we study whether the CEOs with more
masculine faces make the firms more risky and spend more resources in acquisitions to achieve
dominant positions in the industry.
Whether facial masculinity is directly affected by testosterone, a steroid hormone that drives a
person to take more risk to occupy a dominant position in a competitive setting, is still under debate in
the literature. Verdonck, Gaethofs, Carels, and de Zegher (1999), Nie (2005), Thornhill and Møller
(1997), Thornhill and Gangestad (1999), and Lindberg et al. (2005) document that more testosterone
during the pubertal stage makes the facial bone grow to be more masculine with higher fWHR. Pound,
Penton-Voak, and Surridge (2009) find that more masculine-faced males show higher circulating
testosterone level after winning a competitive task than less masculine-faced males do. In contrast,
experimenting with 91 Tsimane tribe adolescents in the South American jungle, Hodges-Simeon,
HansoSobraske, Samore, Gurven, and Gaulin (2016) document that fWHR is not affected by pubertal
testosterone level. However, once they control for the age of the subjects, they still find significant
association between fWHR and testosterone. Moreover, an individual's fWHR does not change
significantly over time, particularly after puberty (Jia et al., 2014). Lefevre, Lewis, Perrett, and Penke
(2013) find that fWHR is significantly positively correlated with both the levels of baseline
(circulating) and reactive testosterone. Additionally, Penton-Voak and Chen (2004) find that males
with facial masculinity have higher testosterone levels in their saliva.
1
Adams, Keloharju, and Knüpfer's (2016) study finds that some traits are related to career success to become CEOs as in
Du, Gao, and Levi (2012).
m
r
(
(
(
A
d
(
n
(
(
(
w
C
c
b
t
R
t
t
t
r
p
p
c
w
o
n
f
m
r
d
2
r
a
3
4
s
5
s
6
L
L
. KAMIYA ET AL.
|
3
241
r
l
y
e
&
,
e
e
&
,
r
h
s
y
h
n
p
.
e
o
y
s
e
e
a
n
r
e
,
g
,
,
l
t
e
e
e
s
The level of testosterone is thought to be associated with the person's behaviors through neural
mechanisms (Dabbs & Mallinger, 1999; Mehta & Beer, 2010). Studies have uncovered that a set of
related behavioral characteristics are associated with testosterone levels. These include aggression
(Archer, 2006), sensation-seeking (Roberti, 2004), hostility (Hartgens & Kuipers, 2004), dominance
(Mazur & Booth, 1998; Sherman, Lerner, Josephs, Renshon, & Gross, 2016), egocentrism
(Eisenegger, Naef, Snozzi, Heinrichs, & Fehr, 2010), and risk-seeking (Apicella et al., 2008).
2
Apicella et al. (2008) find that men with masculine faces as a proxy for higher testosterone exposure
during puberty are more likely to make risky financial decisions. Sapienza, Zingales, and Maestripieri
(2009) and Stenstrom, Saad, Nepomuceno, and Mendenhall (2011) show that risk aversion is
negatively related to prenatal testosterone exposure measured by the ratio of the length of the second
(index) finger to the length of the fourth (ring) finger (2D:4D ratio).
3
Coates, Gurnell, and Rustichini
(2009) find that a lower 2D:4D ratio of male traders delivers better trading performance.
4,5
Our argument is slightly different from proposing more gender diversity as in Huang and Kisgen
(2013), Adams and Ragunathan (2017), or Faccio, Marchica, and Mura (2016). Instead, we study
whether, within the same gender (specifically male) group, the different levels of masculinity of the
CEO drive the firm to be systematically different in terms of risk and dominance, supporting behavioral
consistency theory (Epstein, 1979; Funder & Colvin, 1991). The CEOs of large corporations and the
bankers around the world have been criticized for their masculinity-driven culture, particularly after
the financial crisis in 2008, because of their excessive risk-taking behavior before the crisis (Adams &
Ragunathan, 2017; Sherman, 2012).
6
A recent report by Perman (2012) reveals that testosterone
therapy became popular among Wall Street traders and some corporate CEOs, because it helped them
to be more masculine and be more willing to take risks. Therefore, it is imperative to study the impact of
the masculinity of the CEO on the risk of the firm: Just how much does masculinitymake the firm more
risky?
Based on our hand-collected face pictures of 1,162 unique CEOs in Execucomp (largest 1,500
public US firms) in non-financial industries, our multiple regression analyses show a significantly
positive association between fWHR and the risk of the firm measured by return volatility of the firm
controlling for CEO's overconfidence (Malmendier & Tate, 2005). In addition, to make sure that what
we find is not driven by some extreme values of our explanatory variable, we use alternative measures
of fWHR, such as the inverse rank of fWHR (the highest fWHR CEO having the largest ordinal
number), the dummy variables that are one if the CEO belongs to the highest quantiles of fWHR, and
find consistent results. Moreover, for the first time in the literature, we use artificial intelligence (AI) to
measure the fWHR automatically to steer clear of human error in measuring fWHR and find consistent
results in our robustness check section.
For CEOs to influence the risk of the firm, the channel should be either through the capital structure
decision or the capital budgeting decision (Brealey, Myers, Allen, & Mohanty, 2012). Therefore, we
2
Other studies include Mehta, Jones, and Josephes (2008), Pound et al. (2009), and Zuckerman and Kuhlman (2000) for
risky behavior; Wright et al. (2012) for egocentric behavior. Other studies include Van Honk and Schutter (2007), Wirth
and Schultheiss (2007), and Josephs, Sellers, Newman, and Mehta (2006).
3
Higher (lower) prenatal testosterone causes lower (higher) 2D:4D ratio.
4
Coates, Gurnell, and Sarnyai (2010) summarize their findings and provide an excellent survey of the relationship between
steroid hormones and financial risk-taking.
5
However, whether testosterone is really driving the risk-taking behavior is also debated. Schipper (2014) fails to find a
significant relationship between testosterone and risk aversion in competitive bidding experiments.
6
Some people in the Wall Street Journal noted that Lehman Brothers might have been safer if it was Lehman Sisters or
Lehman Brothers and Sisters (Kristof, 2009).

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT