The Impact of Emotions on Stakeholder Reactions to Organizational Wrongdoing

AuthorArnaud Banoun,Meena Andiappan,Lucas Dufour
Published date01 September 2019
DOIhttp://doi.org/10.1111/emre.12141
Date01 September 2019
The Impact of Emotions on Stakeholder
Reactions to Organizational Wrongdoing
LUCAS DUFOUR,
1
MEENA ANDIAPPAN
1
and ARNAUD BANOUN
2
1
Montpellier Business School, Montpellier, France
2
EDC Paris, France
In this study, we develop a theoretical model thatcaptures the influence of emotions on a stakeholders decision to
act against an organization afterorganizational wrongdoing. Relying oncognitive appraisal theory, we focus on the
role of emotions during two central phases in the stakeholder response process: appraisal and intention. First, we
argue that based on their appraisal of the wrongdoing, stakeholders will experience certain primary emotions
(e.g. disappointment, anger)which will impact their first response towardsthe organization. Second,we propose that
certain secondaryemotions (e.g. hope, remorse)will influence their intentionto act against the organization. Finally,
we argue that the stakeholders perceived power and self-interest will influence the stakeholders decision to act
against the organization.
Keywords: emotions; organizational wrongdoing; stakeholder; cognitive appraisal theory
Introduction
Over the past 30 years, a vast body of literature has
investigated how stakeholders perceive organizational
wrongdoing (for a review see Parmar et al., 2010).
However, it is only recently that studies have begun to
focus on how stakeholders respond to, and take action
against, organizations in the face of wrongdoing (Rhee
and Valdez, 2009; Reuber and Fischer, 2010; Barnett,
2014). This focus was greatly prompted by the string of
corporate scandals exposed by the media over the past
two decades, including such companies as Worldcom,
Tyco and more recently, Volkswagen which engendered
strong stakeholder reactions.
When stakeholders perceive organizational
wrongdoing that is to say, when an organization acts
in a way that does not respect its obligations to
stakeholders they are frequently driven to act against
the organization, with the goal of either punishing the
organization or encouraging it to change (Barnett, 2014).
Surprisingly, the evidence suggests that stakeholders can
react very differently from one another when facing the
exact same organizational wrongdoing; for example,
while some stakeholders remain loyal to the organization,
others definitively end their relationship with the
organization and still others engage in negative word-
of-mouth (Ferguson and Johnston, 2011). The reasons
that have been proposed to explain such a discrepancy
in stakeholdersreactions have primarily focused on
cognitive and rational factors (Reuber and Fischer,
2010; Barnett, 2014). Though numerous authors have
noted that the stakeholders reaction process has been
considered as largely rational and that the role of
emotional factors remain understudied (Coombs and
Holladay, 2004; Gillespie and Dietz, 2009; Rhee and
Hadwick, 2011), it is only recently that researchers have
begun to investigate the role of emotions on
stakeholdersreactions (e.g. Rafaeli and Vilnai-Yavetz,
2004; Siltaoja and Lahdesmaki, 2015).
Our goal in the presentstudy is to understand the role of
emotions in stakeholder responses to organizational
wrongdoing. We focus on how emotions and perceptions
affect stakeholders initial reactions, and subsequent
intentions, to act towards the organization. Thus, our
paper develops a three-part model based on the cognitive
appraisal framework: first, we explore the relationship
between primary emotions and initial reactions to
wrongdoing based on the stakeholders appraisal of the
wrongdoing; second, we explore the relationship between
secondary emoti ons and stakeholder i ntentions; and
finally, we analyze how the stakeholders evaluation of
the resources and opportunities available to him or her
Correspondence: Lucas Dufour, Montpellier Business School, 2300
Avenue des moulins 34185 Montpellier, France. E-mail l.dufour@
montpellier-bs.com
DOI: 10.1111/emre.12141
©2017 European Academy of Management
European Management Review, Vol. 16, 761, (2019 )
7 9
77
stakeholders perceived power and perceived self-interest
influence the s takeholders decision to act against t he
organization.
We believe that for academics and firms alike,
understanding the role of emotions on stakeholder
response after organizational wrongdoing is as critical as
understanding the role of cognitions, for the following
three reasons. First, we believe that the inclusion of
emotions into the study of stakeholder response can shed
light on the paradox of why certain wrongdoings result
in public actions against a firm, while a similar
wrongdoing may result in very little reaction. For
example, if a company like the Body Shop whose image
is centered on social responsibility, trust and ethics
(Fukukawa et al., 2007) faces litigation for employee
exploitation, it is more likely to face difficulties in
regaining stakeholder confidence than a company facing
the same litigation that developed relationships with its
stakeholders based on a low-cost strategy. As Frijda
(1986) broadly noted, emotions are the outcomes of the
process of evaluating the world in termsof an individuals
own concerns and which modify readinessfor action. We
argue that one of the reasons for the above disparity in
stakeholder reactions is due to emotions: stakeholders in
the former case may feel emotions such as anger, while
in the latter case, they are more likely to feel emotions
such as disappointment. Second, studying emotions can
serve to aid firms in formulating their strategic responses
to such wrongdoings. For example, for firms facing
disappointedstakeholders, reorientingstakeholder interest
and refusing to accept responsibility for an event may
work well (a defense or distract strategy). However, for
firms facing angry stakeholders, this type of strategy is
likely to have detrimental consequences; instead, firms
may choose toengage in dialogue with these stakeholders,
and attempt to appease them by listening and responding
to their concerns (thus deescalating the situation). Third,
emotionalreactions guide responsesnot solely at their first
appearance, but also through the secondary reactions to
uncertain situations (Loewenstein et al., 2001). Thus,
emotions may be necessary and even act to mediate the
connection between cognitive evaluations of risk-related
behavior in uncertain situations.
In the past decade, a n umber of studies have
demonstrated that emotions, defined as a mental
experience with high intensity and high hedonic content
(Cabanac, 2002: 80), strongly influence stakeholder
perceptions of organizational actions and decisions
(Coombs, 2007; Kim and Cameron, 2011; Huy et al.,
2014). However, the literature appears scarce on two
fronts. First, very few articles have proposed a theoretical
model that integrate how the emotions experienced by
stakeholders influence their decision to act against an
organization after perceiving an organizational
wrongdoing (with the exception of Ashton-James and
Ashkanasys 2008 work that concentrates on the impac t
of emotions on strategic decisions). Second, the reasons
why two stakeholders experiencedifferent emotions while
facing a similar organizational wrongdoing has not been
clearly investigated.
In this paper, we identify the specific emotions an
individual stakeholder experiences when facing
organizational wrongdoing and how these emotions
influence his or her intention to act against the
organization. In the fo llowing paragraphs, we d efine
organizational wrongdoing and present a brief literature
review of the role of emotions in the response process.
Drawing on the response and intent literature, we
distinguish three steps in the stakeholders response
process: first, their immediate emotional response;
second, their intention to act against an organization and
third their decision to act against the organization. For
each step of the process, we identify the emotions
experienced by the stakeholder that are most likely to
enhance or inhibit the stakeholders intention to act
against an organization, and how the stakeholders
evaluation of the resources and opportunities available
can influence the stakeholdersresponse.
This paper contributes to the management literature by
extending and challenging current theory in two ways.
First, the majority of studies linking emotions to
stakeholder actions in terms of wrongdoing have focused
on the direct impactof anger on the action (e.g. Lerner and
Tiedens, 2006; Coombs, 2007). Based on the cognitive
appraisal model of emotions framework, we suggest that
in addition to ange r, three other primar y emotions
(disappointment, fear and disgust) can trigger the
stakeholdersaction against an organizationand that these
emotions are moderated by secondary emotions (hope,
fear of counter-retaliation, remorse and righteous anger).
Second, for both primary and secondary emotions, we
specify which emotions are likely to occur at each phas e
in the response process, how they affect intent, and why.
In this way, we respond to the calls made by studies such
as Lerner et al. (2015) askingfor management literature to
investigate further the crucial issues of emotions,
intentions and actions.
Definition of organizational wrongdoing
Organizational wrongdoing can be defined as any illegal,
unethical, unfairor potentially harmful actionthat violates
the stakeholders expectations and that encourages the
stakeholder to take action to stop, prevent or punish the
organization involved (Miceli et al., 2012). According to
Palmer (2012), there are two different approaches that
can be used to explain the causes of organizational
wrongdoing. The first approach, considered as the
dominant framewor k, assumes that wrongdo ing is
produced by either bad apples(rational actors that
L. Dufour et al.
©2017 European Academy of Management
762

To continue reading

Request your trial

VLEX uses login cookies to provide you with a better browsing experience. If you click on 'Accept' or continue browsing this site we consider that you accept our cookie policy. ACCEPT